|
|
Cultivating Positive Emotions
to Optimize Health and Well-Being
Barbara L.
Fredrickson University of Michigan
ABSTRACT
This article develops the hypothesis
that intervention strategies that cultivate positive emotions
are particularly suited for preventing and treating problems rooted
in negative emotions, such as anxiety, depression, aggression,
and stress-related health problems. Fredrickson's (1998) broaden–and–build
model of positive emotions provides the foundation for this application.
According to this model, the form and function of positive and
negative emotions are distinct and complementary. Negative emotions
(e.g., fear, anger, and sadness) narrow an individual's momentary
thought–action repertoire toward specific actions that served
the ancestral function of promoting survival. By contrast, positive
emotions (e.g., joy, interest, and contentment) broaden an individual's
momentary thought–action repertoire, which in turn can build that
individual's enduring personal resources, resources that also
served the ancestral function of promoting survival. One implication of the broaden–and–build model is that positive
emotions have an undoing effect on negative emotions. By broadening
the momentary thought–action repertoire, positive emotions loosen
the hold that negative emotions gain on an individual's mind and
body by undoing the narrowed psychological and physiological preparation
for specific action. Indeed, empirical studies have shown that
contentment and joy speed recovery from the cardiovascular aftereffects
of negative emotions (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998). Stepping off from these ideas and findings,
a range of intervention and coping strategies are reviewed, including
relaxation therapies, behavioral therapies aimed at increasing
rates of pleasant activities, cognitive therapies aimed at teaching
optimism, and coping strategies marked by finding positive meaning.
These strategies optimize health and well-being to the extent
that they cultivate positive emotions. Cultivated positive emotions
not only counteract negative emotions, but also broaden individuals'
habitual modes of thinking and build their personal resources
for coping.
Experiences
of negative emotion are inevitable and at times useful. Even
so, when extreme, prolonged, or contextually inappropriate, negative
emotions can trigger a wide array of problems for individuals
and for society. Fear and anxiety, for instance, fuel phobias
and other anxiety disorders (Ohman, 1993) and together with acute and chronic stress may
compromise immune functioning and create susceptibilities to stress-related
physical disorders ( O'Leary, 1990). For some individuals, sadness
and grief may swell into unipolar depression
(Nolen-Hoeksema, Morrow, & Fredrickson, 1993), which when severe
can lead to immunosuppression (O'Leary,
1990), loss of work productivity (Coryell, Scheftner,
Keller, & Endicott, 1993), and suicide (
Chen & Dilsaver, 1996). Anger
and its poor management have been implicated in the etiology of
heart disease (Barefoot, Dahlstrom, & Williams, 1983; Fredrickson, Maynard, et
al., 1999; Scheier & Bridges, 1995;
Williams et al., 1980) and some cancers ( Eysenck,
1994; Greer & Morris, 1975), as well as in aggression and
violence, especially in boys and men ( Buss, 1994; Lemerise
& Dodge, 1993).
Given
the suffering and loss that stem from negative emotions, the press
to understand these emotions is immense. In part reflecting this
press, the scientific literature on emotions includes far more
publications on negative emotions, like fear, anger, and sadness,
than on positive emotions, like joy, interest, and contentment.
One could argue that efforts to understand positive emotion should
be postponed while psychologists learn more about preventing and
treating the disease and suffering caused by negative emotions.
But what if positive emotions could help to solve some of the
problems that negative emotions generate? What if positive emotions
could help people overcome negative emotions faster and build
their resilience to future adversities?
This
article takes these possibilities seriously. I begin by presenting
a model that describes the form and function of positive emotions
(Fredrickson, 1998). I next provide a brief overview of studies
that test the implications of this model for the role of positive
emotions in regulating negative emotions ( Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998;
Fredrickson, Mancuso, Branigan, &
Tugade, 1999). My ultimate aim is to illustrate the implications
that this new model has for counteracting—both preventing and
treating—individual and societal problems that stem from negative
emotions. This is not a tautological endeavor: Preventing or alleviating
problematic negative emotions does not in itself cultivate positive
emotions. Positive emotions are more than the absence of negative
emotions.
The capacity to experience
positive emotions remains a largely untapped human strength. The
possible benefits of positive emotions seem particularly undervalued
in cultures like ours that endorse the Protestant ethic, which
casts hard work and self-discipline as virtues and leisure and
pleasures as sinful. Departing from this ethic, I will argue that
the best solutions to problems stemming from negative emotions
are ones that capitalize on positive emotions. An important feature
of positive emotions is that their effects do not end once suffering
is prevented or alleviated. The repercussions of experiencing
positive emotions resonate further: I hypothesize that positive
emotions, when tapped effectively, can optimize health, subjective
well-being, and psychological resilience. This outlook concurs
with the emerging view that psychology should examine, both theoretically
and empirically, the positive aspects of human experience as rigorously
as it does the negative aspects ( Ryff & Singer, 1998; Seligman, 1998).
Current
Perspectives on Emotion
A
brief review of current perspectives on emotions provides an
important backdrop. Working definitions of emotions vary somewhat
among researchers. Even so, a consensus is emerging that emotions
are multi- component response tendencies that unfold over relatively
short time spans. Typically, an emotion process begins with
an individual's assessment of the personal meaning of some antecedent
event—what Lazarus (1991) called the "person–environment
relationship," or "adaptational
encounter." This appraisal process triggers a cascade of
response tendencies, which may be manifest across loosely coupled
component systems, such as subjective experience, facial expressions,
and physiological changes. Emotions differ from moods in that
they are about some personally meaningful circumstance (i.e.,
they have an object), whereas moods are often free-floating
or objectless (Oatley & Jenkins,
1996). Emotions also differ from affective traits, such as hostility,
neuroticism, or optimism: Enduring affective traits predispose
individuals toward experiencing certain emotions, and so affective
traits and emotional states represent different levels of analysis
(Rosenberg, 1998).
Current
models of emotion are typically intended to explain emotions in
general. Despite this aim, most models are built to the specifications
of prototypic negative emotions (e.g., anger and fear) with positive
emotions (e.g., joy and contentment) squeezed in later, as an
afterthought. As one critical example, key to many models of emotions,
is the idea that emotions are, by definition, associated with
specific action tendencies (Frijda,
1986; Frijda, Kuipers,
& Schure, 1989; Lazarus, 1991; Levenson, 1994; Oatley & Jenkins,
1996; Tooby & Cosmides,
1990). Fear, for example, is linked with the urge to escape, anger
with the urge to attack, disgust the urge to expel, and so on.
No theorist would argue that people invariably act out these urges
when feeling particular emotions. But rather, people's ideas about
possible courses of action narrow in on a specific set of behavioral
options. A key idea in these models is that having these specific
action tendencies come to mind is what makes emotions evolutionarily
adaptive: These are among the actions that worked best in getting
our ancestors out of life-or-death situations. Another key idea
is that specific action tendencies and physiological changes go
hand- in-hand. So, for example, when you have an urge to escape
when feeling fear, your body reacts by mobilizing appropriate
autonomic support for the possibility of running (Levenson, 1992, 1994).
Fredrickson
and Levenson (1998) noted that negative
and positive emotions are not isomorphic in this regard. Instead,
the specific action tendencies identified for positive emotions
are vague and underspecified. Joy, for instance, is linked to
aimless activation, contentment with inactivity, and interest
with attending ( Frijda,
1986). These tendencies, in our view, are far too general to be
called specific. This is an example of how theorists have tended
to squeeze positive emotions into the same theoretical mold as
negative emotions. Whereas others before me have noted that the
fit for positive emotions is poor (Ekman, 1992; Lazarus, 1991), to my reading of the literature,
this acknowledgment had not yet been productive. The need for
a better way to make sense of positive emotions was clear.
Taking Positive Emotions Seriously
The
first question is: If many positive emotions do not share the
hallmark feature with the negative emotions of promoting and
supporting specific actions, then what is their form and possible
function? To answer this, ideas about positive emotions need
to be uncoupled from ideas about negative emotions. There is
good reason to retain models based on specific action tendencies
for negative emotions but to start fresh for positive emotions.
In
making this fresh start, I propose discarding two common presumptions
(Fredrickson, 1998). The first is that emotions must necessarily
yield specific action tendencies. Although positive emotions often
produce urges to act, they appear to be less prescriptive than
negative emotions about which particular actions should be taken.
The second is that emotions must necessarily spark tendencies
for physical action. Some of the positive emotions seem instead
to spark changes primarily in cognitive activity. So, in place
of action tendencies, I refer to thought–action tendencies
. Additionally, instead of presuming
these thought–action tendencies are specific, I discuss the relative
breadth of the momentary thought–action repertoire.
Using
this new terminology, traditional action-oriented models can be
paraphrased for negative emotions as follows: Negative emotions
narrow a person's momentary thought–action repertoire. They do
so by calling to mind and body the time-tested, ancestrally adaptive
actions represented by specific action tendencies. This effect
is clearly adaptive in life-threatening situations that require
quick action to survive. Because positive emotions are not linked
to threats requiring quick action, an alternative model seems
warranted: I have proposed that positive emotions broaden a person's
momentary thought–action repertoire (Fredrickson, 1998). In the
following sections, I build the case for this proposal by describing
three distinct positive emotions: joy, interest, and contentment.
For each, I touch on (a) the circumstances
that tend to elicit the emotion (b) apparent changes in the momentary
thought– action repertoire, and (c) the consequences or outcomes
of these changes.
Joy
Joy
arises in contexts appraised as safe and familiar (Izard, 1977),
as requiring low effort (Ellsworth & Smith, 1988), and in
some cases, by events construed as accomplishments or progress
toward one's goals (Izard, 1977; Lazarus, 1991). Frijda (1986) offered the clearest statement on the action
tendency associated with joy, which he termed free activation:
"[it] is in part aimless, unasked-for readiness to engage
in whatever interaction presents itself and in part readiness
to engage in enjoyments" (p. 89). In other words, joy creates
the urge to play and be playful in the broadest sense of the word,
encompassing not only physical and social play, but also intellectual
and artistic play. Play, especially imaginative play, is to a
large degree unscripted. It involves exploration, invention and
just plain fooling around. Pointing to no single set of actions,
play takes many forms. To my mind, then, the urge to play represents
a quite generic, nonspecific thought–action tendency. Joy and
related positive emotions (e.g., exhilaration and amusement) can
thus be described as broadening an individual's thought–action
repertoire.
Even
though play is often aimless, it does appear to have reliable
outcomes. Certainly, social play builds and strengthens friendships
and attachments. In addition, ethologists have long argued that play promotes skill acquisition:
Physical skills are developed and practiced in rough-and-tumble
play, manipulative–cognitive skills are developed and practiced
in object play, and social–affective skills are developed and
practiced in social play (Boulton & Smith, 1992; Dolhinow
& Bishop, 1970). More recently, Panksepp
(1998) proposed that childhood play drives brain development,
especially in the frontal lobes responsible for executive functions
and implicated in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Joy, then, not only broadens an individual's momentary thought–action
repertoire through the urge to play, but also, over time and
as a product of recurrent play, can have the incidental effect
of building an individual's physical, intellectual, and social
resources. Importantly, these new resources are durable, and
can be drawn on later, long after the instigating experience
of joy has subsided.
Interest
Interest, Izard (1977) proposed,
is the emotion experienced most frequently. Interest and related
affective states (e.g., curiosity, wonder, excitement, intrinsic
motivation, and flow) arise in contexts appraised as safe and
as offering novelty, change, a sense of possibility (Izard, 1977),
challenge ( Csikszentmihalyi, 1990),
or mystery (Kaplan, 1992). These contexts also tend to be appraised
as important and requiring effort and attention (Ellsworth &
Smith, 1988). Some theorists have posited that the momentary thought–action
tendency of interest is to simply attend (e.g., Frijda,
1986). Yet to my mind this stops short of fully describing the
impact of interest. Instead, I favor Izard's
(1977) treatment of interest, which builds on earlier work by
Tomkins (1962). The momentary thought–action
tendency sparked by interest, according to Izard (1977), is exploration,
explicitly and actively aimed at increasing knowledge of and experience
with the target of interest. Interest generates "a feeling
of wanting to investigate, become involved, or extend or expand
the self by incorporating new information and having new experiences
with the person or object that has stimulated the interest"
(Izard, 1977, p. 216). Although interest may or may not be accompanied
by overt physical action, it is nonetheless associated with feeling
animated and enlivened; Tomkins (1962)
characterized interest as thinking with excitement.
Importantly, the openness to new ideas, experiences, and
actions is what characterizes the mindset of interest as broadened,
rather than narrowed.
Although
interested individuals explore for intrinsic reasons, to satisfy
their own inner curiosity, such exploration has reliable outcomes.
Most obviously, interest-inspired exploration increases an individual's
knowledge (Deci, Vallerand, Pelletier, &
Ryan, 1991; Hazen & Durrett, 1982;
Renninger, Hidi, & Krapp, 1992). Beyond simply incrementing knowledge, interest
and related states also appear to foster "psychological
complexity," defined by Csikszentmihalyi
and Rathunde (1998) as the ability to integrate and differentiate
complex relationships with people and among concepts and strivings.
Similarly, Izard (1977), again building on Tomkins
(1962), wrote that interest is the primary instigator of personal
growth, creative endeavor, and development of intelligence.
Interest, then, not only broadens an individual's momentary
thought–action repertoire as the individual is enticed to explore,
but over time and as a product of sustained exploration, interest
also builds the individual's store of knowledge and cognitive
abilities. Again, these become durable resources that can be
accessed in later moments, and in other emotional states.
Contentment
Contentment
and related emotions (e.g., serenity, tranquillity,
and relief) arise in situations appraised as safe and as having
a high degree of certainty and a low degree of effort (Ellsworth
& Smith, 1988). This emotion is distinct from mere satisfaction,
or the pleasure that derives from a good meal or otherwise meeting
bodily needs. It may also be the positive emotion least appreciated
in Western cultures. In part, contentment is captured by the Japanese
emotion term amae, which refers
to the sense of being accepted and cared for by others in a passive
relationship of reciprocal dependence (Markus & Kitayama,
1991).
At first blush, to the extent
that inactivity is not an action, contentment appears to have
no real action tendency. It may be, however, that the changes
sparked by contentment are more cognitive than physical. A closer
look at theoretical writings on contentment and related states
suggests that this emotion prompts individuals to savor the moment
or recent experiences, feel "oneness" with others or
the world around them, and integrate current and recent experiences
into their overall self-concept and world view (Izard, 1977; de
Rivera, Possel, Verette, & Weiner, 1989).
Contentment is not simply behavioral passivity but rather a reflective
broadening of a person's self-views and world view.
Contentment,
according to this analysis, is a mindful emotion. It involves
full awareness of, and openness to momentary experiences; it carries
the urge to savor and integrate those experiences, which in turn
creates a new sense of self and a new world view. These links
to mindfulness, receptivity, integration, self-complexity, and
insight characterize contentment as an emotion that broadens individuals'
momentary thought–action repertoires, and builds their personal
resources.
Positive
Emotions Broaden and Build
A
parallelism has emerged here: Not only do joy, interest, and
contentment share the feature of broadening an individual's
momentary thought–action repertoire, but they also share the
feature of building the individual's personal resources, ranging
from physical and intellectual resources to social resources.
Importantly, these resources are more durable than the transient
emotional states that led to their acquisition. By consequence,
then, the often incidental effect of experiencing a positive
emotion is an increment in enduring personal resources that
can be drawn on later, in other contexts and in other emotional
states. I call this the broaden–and–build model of positive
emotions (Fredrickson, 1998).
A wide range of empirical evidence
supports specific predictions drawn from the broaden–and–build
model: Positive emotions and related positive states have been
linked to broadened scopes of attention, cognition, and action
and enhanced physical, intellectual and social resources (for
a review, see Fredrickson, 1998 ). The work of Alice Isen and her colleagues is exemplary: Their experiments have
demonstrated that positive emotions produce patterns of thought
that are notably unusual (Isen, Johnson,
Mertz, & Robinson, 1985), flexible (Isen
& Daubman, 1984), creative (Isen,
Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987), and
receptive (Estrada, Isen, & Young,
1997 ). In general terms, Isen suggested
that positive affect "enlarges the cognitive context"
(Isen, 1987, p. 222), an effect recently linked to increases
in brain dopamine levels ( Ashby, Isen,
& Turken, in press). This body of
evidence is consistent with the broadening effects of positive
emotions that I have proposed. Importantly, the broaden–and–build
model does not predict that persons experiencing positive emotions
become unfocused, scattered, or shallow thinkers. In contrast,
it suggests that these persons typically maintain their focus
within the emotion-relevant domain (e.g., the specific love relationship,
playful episode, or target of interest), but are at the same time
generative of, or receptive to, a wide range of ideas and actions
within the domain of their focus.
Moreover,
positive emotions have relational repercussions. Even though positive
emotions broaden thought–action repertoires within individuals,
such broadening can impact interpersonal relationships, especially
enduring ones. Observations of married couples, for instance,
reveal that the interaction patterns of unhappy couples are structured,
predictable, and rigid. Happy couples, by contrast, interact in
more unpredictable ways (Gottman, 1998),
a finding consistent with the proposed broadening effects of positive
emotions. In addition, Gottman (1998)
contended that members of happy couples build up a surplus (or
"bank account") of positive sentiments for their partner
and their marriage. Over time, this surplus functions as a social
resource: Couples that have it are less likely to escalate each
other's negative emotions when faced with conflict. Here again
the analysis is consistent with the proposal that positive emotions
build enduring social resources.
An Evolutionary Functional Analysis
of Positive Emotions
Before
taking up the implications of this new model, I would like to
discuss it in the context of human evolution. One route to arguing
that a particular psychological phenomenon is an evolved adaptation
is to take a form-to-function approach: First one notes
the form of some existing psychological phenomenon, then thinks
back to the lives of our hunter–gatherer ancestors and tries
to locate the sort of adaptive problem that might have been
solved by this form (Tooby & Cosmides,
1992). The form that characterizes positive emotions, I have
argued, is a momentarily broadened thought–action repertoire.
The common denominator across the contexts that elicit positive
emotions is perceived safety and satiation. The ability to recognize
and take advantage of the opportunities inherent in safe and
satiated moments is, at face value, of obvious importance. Of
all the things a hunter– gatherer could do in a such a moment—sleep,
sit around, continue to run, attack, be vigilant—why might being
playful or exploratory have led to a reproductive advantage?
The
key is in the "build" part of the broaden– and–build
model. Through the experiences of positive emotions, ancestors
built their personal resources, including physical resources (e.g.,
the ability to outmaneuver a predator), intellectual resources
(e.g.,a detailed
cognitive map for wayfinding), and social
resources (e.g., someone to turn to for help). These links between
positive emotions and resource building suggest that positive
emotions may be essential to early child development. Indeed,
Panksepp (1998) argued that "youth
may have evolved to give complex organisms time to play"
(p. 96). Importantly, the personal resources accrued during positive
states were durable. When these same ancestors later faced threats
to life and limb, these resources could translate themselves into
increased odds of survival, and in turn, increased odds of living
long enough to reproduce. Thus, the adaptive problem that appears
to be solved by positive emotions is this: When and how should
individuals build resources for survival? The answer is to build
resources during safe and satiated moments by playing, exploring,
or savoring and integrating. Together with this evolutionary functional
analysis, the broaden–and–build model describes what positive
emotions have been good for—their ancestral function—and explains
why they are now part of our universal human nature.
The evolutionary functional
analysis I have sketched does not mean that experiences of positive
emotions necessarily have adaptive advantages in present day circumstances,
nor that present-day humans pursue positive emotions to maximize
their odds of survival, reproduction, or inclusive fitness. Indeed,
positive emotions may now serve multiple purposes in people's
lives. At times, the "pursuit of happiness" may solely
reflect the fact that positive emotions are hedonically pleasant
and therefore inherently rewarding. Present-day motivations aside,
the adaptationist account I offer makes
the more modest claim that the structure and effects of positive
emotions evident in present-day humans have been shaped by the
recurrent conditions faced by our ancestors over the course of
human evolution.
Although
we cannot assume that positive emotions inevitably "do good,"
the insights that the broaden–and–build model offers into the
psychological form and ancestral function of positive emotions
can illuminate ways that present-day humans might deploy positive
emotions to optimize health and well-being. Moreover, the broaden–
and–build model claims that positive emotions can have effects
beyond making people "feel good" or improving their
subjective experiences of life. They also have the potential to
broaden people's habitual modes of thinking and build their physical,
intellectual, and social resources. These processes, I will argue,
can help people overcome current stresses faster and make them
more resilient to future adversities.
Implications for Emotion Regulation:
The Undoing Effect of Positive Emotions
I
have argued that positive emotions broaden individuals' momentary
thought–action repertoires. If true, then positive emotions should
also serve as particularly effective antidotes for the lingering
effects of negative emotions, which narrow individuals' thought–action
repertoires. In other words, positive emotions should have an
undoing effect on negative emotions. The basic observation that
positive emotions (or their key components) are somehow incompatible
with negative emotions is not new, and has been demonstrated over
several decades by a range of researchers working on affect-related
processes (Baron, 1976; Cabanac, 1971;
Nezu, Nezu,
& Blissett, 1988; Solomon, 1980
; Wolpe, 1958). Even so, the
precise mechanism or mechanisms ultimately responsible for this
long-noted incompatibility have not been adequately identified.
Broadening
may turn out to be the operative mechanism. By broadening the
momentary thought–action repertoire, positive emotions may loosen
the hold that (no longer relevant) negative emotions gain on an
individual's mind and body by dismantling or undoing the narrowed
psychological and physiological preparation for specific action.
I propose, then, that the broadened thought–action repertoire
of positive emotions is psychologically incompatible with the
narrowed thought–action repertoire of negative emotions. In addition,
to the extent that a negative emotion's narrowed thought–action
repertoire (i.e., specific action tendency) evokes physiological
changes to support the indicated action (Levenson,
1994), a counteracting positive emotion—with its broadened thought–action
repertoire—should quell or undo this physiological preparation
for specific action. By returning the body to baseline levels
of physiological activation, positive emotions create physiological
support for pursuing the wider array of thoughts and actions called
forth.
Building
on this reasoning, my colleagues and I hypothesized that positive
emotions should have a unique ability to down-regulate the lingering
cardiovascular aftereffects of negative emotions. We tested
this aspect of the undoing hypothesis in a series of experiments
(Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998;
Fredrickson, Mancuso, et al., 1999). Our empirical strategy
was first to induce negative emotional arousal in all participants,
using either a fear- eliciting film clip (Fredrickson &
Levenson, 1998) or an anxiety- eliciting
speech task (Fredrickson, Mancuso, et al., 1999). Next, into
this context of negative emotional arousal (and using a between-groups
design), we induced amusement, contentment, neutrality, or sadness,
again using film clips. We tested our hypothesis by measuring
how long it took for the initial negative emotional arousal
to return to baseline levels once the randomly assigned secondary
film was introduced. Across three independent samples, we found
that the two positive emotion films— the amusement film and
the contentment film—each accelerated cardiovascular recovery
relative to the neutral and sad films (Fredrickson & Levenson,
1998, Study 1; Fredrickson, Mancuso, et al., 1999). We obtained
further evidence for the undoing effect from a correlational
study that linked spontaneous smiles during negative emotional
arousal to faster cardiovascular recovery from that arousal
(Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998, Study 2).
Beyond speeding physiological
recovery, the hypothesized undoing effect implies that positive
emotions should counteract any aspect of negative emotions that
stems from a narrowed thought–action repertoire. For instance,
negative emotions can entrain people toward narrowed lines of
thinking consistent with the specific action tendencies they trigger.
When angry, individuals may dwell on getting revenge or getting
even; when anxious or afraid, they may dwell on escaping or avoiding
harm; when sad or depressed, they may dwell on the repercussions
of what has been lost. The undoing hypothesis predicts that positive
emotions should restore flexible thinking in these circumstances.
No experiments have yet tested this prediction. Even so, indirect
supportive evidence can be drawn from a collection of correlational
studies. Individuals who express or report higher levels of positive
emotion show more constructive and flexible coping, more abstract
and long-term thinking, and greater emotional distance following
stressful negative events (Keltner &
Bonanno, 1997; Lyubomirsky
& Tucker, 1998; Martin, Kuiper,
Olinger, & Dance, 1993; Stein, Folkman,
Trabasso, & Richards, 1997).
Experiments
have thus documented that positive emotions can undo the cardiovascular
reactivity that lingers following a negative emotion and that
this undoing effect is both reliable and generalizable
( Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Fredrickson, Mancuso, et al., 1999). Importantly,
the evidence suggests that two different positive emotions—contentment
and amusement—although distinct in their phenomenology, share
the ability to undo negative emotional arousal. Moreover, correlational evidence suggests that the undoing effect may
extend beyond speeding physiological recovery. Positive emotions
may also undo the psychological or cognitive narrowing engendered
by negative emotions. Although additional studies are still needed,
I suspect that the undoing effect occurs because positive emotions
broaden people's momentary thought– action repertoires in a manner
that is incompatible with the continuance of negative emotion.
Implications for Preventing and Treating Problems Rooted
in Negative Emotions
I
started this article by specifying individual and social problems
that stem from excessive, prolonged, or contextually inappropriate
negative emotions, ranging from anxiety disorders and depression
to heart disease and aggression. Positive emotions, when channeled
into effective prevention, treatment, and coping strategies,
should be especially effective for counteracting these problems.
The broaden–and–build model, together with the existing evidence
for the undoing effect, provides the basis for this claim. In
this latter half of this article, I take up a range of intervention
and change strategies. These include relaxation therapies, behavioral
therapies aimed at increasing rates of pleasant activities,
cognitive therapies aimed at teaching optimistic explanatory
styles, and coping strategies marked by finding positive meaning
within and despite adversity. My ultimate aim is to illustrate
how positive emotions infuse each of these change strategies
and account for their effectiveness.
It bears underscoring that
the view of positive emotions inherent in the broaden–and–build
model suggests that intervention strategies that cultivate positive
emotions are not simply methods for treating and preventing disease
and distress. Health and well- being are more than the absence
of disease and distress, just as positive emotions are more than
the absence of negative emotions. Taking these truisms to heart,
the intervention strategies discussed below are perhaps best conceptualized
as optimizing health and well-being. In other words, their effects
are likely to go beyond treating and preventing problems that
stem from negative emotions and into the realm of building personal
strength, resilience, and wellness.
Relaxation Therapies
There
is no single relaxation therapy. Instead, there are multiple,
seemingly disparate relaxation practices, ranging from more traditional
forms, like meditation and yoga, originating in India and Asia,
to more modern forms, like progressive muscle relaxation and biofeedback,
developed in the West. Despite obvious dissimilarities across
these forms, empirical studies have shown that each form produces
relaxation and effectively treats problems rooted in, or exacerbated
by, negative emotions, including anxiety disorders (Kabat-Zinn
et al., 1992; Miller, Fletcher, & Kabat-Zinn,
1995 ), as well as headaches, chronic pain, essential hypertension
(Blumenthal, 1985), and day-to-day stress and depression ( Shapiro,
Schwartz, & Bonner, 1998; for reviews see Kabat-Zinn,
1990; J. C. Smith, 1990). For this reason, they are often considered
a single class of treatments. Treatments centered on relaxation
persist for practical reasons: They work. Even so, the mechanisms
or active ingredients responsible for their effectiveness remain
unknown (Blumenthal, 1985).
Are relaxation therapies
contentment therapies?
I propose that relaxation therapies are effective because, at
one level or another, they cultivate the positive emotion of contentment.
Contentment, I have argued, is a mindful emotion; the changes it sparks are more cognitive than
physical. It carries the urge not only to savor the moment but
also to integrate those momentary experiences into an enriched
appreciation of one's place in the world. In abstract terms, contentment
broadens a person's momentary thought–action repertoire. Relaxation
therapies induce key components of contentment, and in doing so,
create conditions for experiencing contentment. As such, the efficacy
of relaxation therapies may derive from the undoing effect of
positive emotions. As we have seen, empirical tests of the undoing
effect confirm that contentment induced by a short film can speed
cardiovascular recovery from laboratory-induced fear and anxiety
(Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998; Fredrickson,
Mancuso, et al., 1999; see also Ulrich et al., 1991, on similar
effects attributed to nature scenes). The contentment produced
by relaxation therapy may similarly undo the real-life anxiety
or stress associated with presenting clinical problems.
I
will build a case for the hypothesis that relaxation therapies
capitalize on contentment by describing components of different
relaxation therapies that resemble components of either contentment
or emotion induction techniques, or both. The emphasis on components
is noteworthy. Contentment, like any emotion, cannot be instilled
directly. People cannot simply will themselves to feel content.
All emotion induction techniques are by necessity indirect.
They typically focus on one component of the more complex emotion
system: a situation or recalled event, a facial expression,
or a mode of thinking. Laboratory research on emotions has shown
that cultivating key components of an emotion can often initiate,
or jump-start, the entire, multicomponent
emotion process. Various forms of relaxation therapy, I argue,
can be reconceptualized as various methods for inducing contentment.
Imagery
exercises. Consider first the thematic imagery exercises central
to many different relaxation therapies. Individuals receiving
relaxation training might be instructed to cultivate an "image
of a quiet beach, or a grassy plain, or a cool mountain top, or
a peaceful pond," (J. C. Smith, 1990, p. 56)or whatever setting
is most relaxing to them at the moment, letting the scene become
as vivid and real as possible. Nature settings seem to be invoked
most often. This is perhaps no coincidence. Nature settings are
especially effective in captivating people's attention and quelling
tension ( Kaplan, 1992, 1995; Orians
& Heerwagen, 1992; Ulrich et al., 1991). Put differently, certain
nature scenes evoke contentment. Other than nature scenes, some
imagery exercises ask people to focus on a childhood triumph or
a recent good experience (J. C. Smith, 1990). The implicit aim
seems to be to encourage people to imagine or relive a pleasant
event and savor it.
Highly
similar imagery exercises and relived emotion tasks have long
been used as induction techniques in laboratory studies of emotion
(e.g., Ekman, Levenson,
& Friesen, 1983; Futterman, Kemeny,
Shapiro, & Fahey, 1994; Levenson, Carstensen, Friesen, &
Ekman, 1991) Method acting also uses
similar techniques: To portray an emotion with conviction, Stanislavski
(1965) taught, actors need to experience the emotion portrayed.
To do this, they should imagine a specific emotion- eliciting
event from their own lives. Imagery exercises—whether used in
relaxation therapy, laboratory studies, or on stage—focus people's
attention on emotion-eliciting situations, and in doing so increase
the probability that experiences of the targeted emotion will
follow. In relaxation therapies, the images called forth are ones
known to activate contentment.
Muscle
exercises. Another component of some relaxation therapies
that resembles an emotion induction technique is progressive muscle
relaxation (PMR, also called isometric squeeze techniques). PMR
was developed by Jacobson (1938) to combat anxiety, incorporated
into Wolpe's (1958) systematic desensitization
therapy, and later simplified by Bernstein and Borkovec (1973). In practice, some form of PMR is often used
to initiate relaxation training sessions (J. C. Smith, 1990
). In these techniques, individuals are asked to tense
and then relax different muscle groups (e.g., hands, arms, back,
or shoulders). The aim of PMR is to reduce overall muscle tension
and action readiness.
Laboratory
techniques for inducing emotion also sometimes target muscles,
although typically those of the face. For instance, in one technique
participants are instructed to contract, one-by-one, the set of
facial muscles that create specific facial expressions of emotion
(Directed Facial Action, or DFA; Ekman et al., 1983; Levenson, Ekman, & Friesen, 1990). (For other nonreactive
methods of eliciting specific facial muscle contractions, see
Larsen, Kasimatis, & Frey, 1992; and Strack,
Martin, & Stepper, 1988.) Other techniques target arm muscles
(Cacioppo, Priester, & Berntson, 1993) or overall body posture (Stepper & Strack, 1993). The logic of these techniques is that the muscle
action associated with a particular emotion can trigger other
components of that emotion. For instance, when targeted facial
muscles are contracted accurately, individuals report feeling
the targeted emotion in 66% of the trials, and exhibit emotion-specific
autonomic responding in 73% of the trials (Levenson
et al., 1990). Thus, experiments document that posed muscle contractions
that mirror those present during emotional states can induce specific
emotions.
Building
on this empirical evidence, I speculate that the dynamic tension–release
sequences of PMR might also induce emotion, namely contentment.
These sequences appear to mimic and extend the muscular activity
associated with intense laughter. Intense laughter involves the
contraction of many muscles in the face, trunk, and limbs, followed
by abrupt tension reduction in these same muscles (Ruch, 1993). Moreover, the tension–release sequences of laughter
give way to "a relaxed posture and typically lowered muscle
tone, associated with a reduced readiness to respond . . . with
planned behavior" (Ruch, 1993,
p. 609). Put differently, a good (joyful) laugh can give way to
relaxed contentment. So perhaps because it mirrors intense laughter,
PMR induces a key component of contentment: an overall reduction
in muscle tension and action readiness.
Meditation
exercises. Many forms of relaxation therapy also use meditation
exercises to cultivate mindfulness, or
full moment-to-moment awareness ( Alexander, Langer, Newman, Chandler,
& Davies, 1989; Kabat-Zinn, 1990;
J. C. Smith, 1990). Meditation practices come in many forms, but
typically individuals are instructed to practice focusing their
attention on the present moment, observing the world and their
own thoughts and feelings in a patient, nonjudgmental way, without
getting caught up in the past or future, or any single line of
thinking or preconceived notion. Cultivating mindfulness also
entails cultivating nonstriving (Kabat-Zinn,
1990) or passivity (J. C. Smith, 1990), the ability to stop unnecessary
goal-directed activity and relinquish unnecessary control. Said
differently, mindfulness involves being, not doing (Kabat-Zinn,
1990). Mindfulness meditation also hinges on cultivating receptivity,
or the ability to trust and accept new experiences (J. C. Smith,
1990).
Descriptions
of the mindfulness gained through meditation strongly resemble
the cognitive components of contentment. As previously described,
contentment produces behavioral passivity (nonstriving),
accompanied by urges to savor the moment and forge new connections.
Importantly, mindfulness is not synonymous with contentment.
Instead, mindfulness creates conditions for contentment to develop.
According to Kabat-Zinn (1990), the
moment-to-moment awareness of mindfulness leads directly to
new ways of seeing: "You see more, and you see more deeply.
You may start seeing an intrinsic order and connectedness between
things that were not apparent before..." (p. 28). This
way of seeing, I propose, sets the stage for contentment and
its associated urges to savor and integrate moment-to-moment
experience.
Comparison
to prior explanations for relaxation therapies
. I have proposed that relaxation
therapies work because they elicit contentment and thus capitalize
on the undoing effects of positive emotion. How does this analysis
compare to earlier explanations for the efficacy of relaxation
therapies? One traditional explanation appeals to the physiological
components of relaxation: Various relaxation therapies were thought
to work because each induces what has been called the "relaxation
response," a collection of physiological changes involving
decreases in heart rate, blood pressure, breathing rate, and muscle
tension (Benson, 1975). This relaxation response was deemed therapeutic
because it is physiologically incompatible with the health-damaging
"stress response" identified earlier by Selye
(1956). Note that this explanation emerged from an era of psychology
that did not favor reference to cognitive or emotional states.
Another, more recent explanation appeals to the cognitive components
of relaxation: Various relaxation therapies are thought to work
because each cultivates mindfulness or cognitive coping skills
that can counteract anxious tendencies (Blumenthal, 1985; Kabat-Zinn,
1990; J. C. Smith, 1990). Recall that emotions have multiple components, spanning both physiological
and cognitive. Thus, conceptualizing contentment and its
associated broadening as the critical ingredient of relaxation
therapies does not necessarily replace previous explanations; it expands
on them. Prior explanations have emphasized only part of the picture:
Either arousal reduction or mindfulness. Full relaxation seems
to involve more.
I
suggest that the therapeutic benefits of relaxation training are
best conceptualized in emotion-related terms: The broadened thought–action
repertoire of contentment is incompatible with the narrowed thought–action
repertoire of negative emotions. Relaxation therapies use a range
of techniques to elicit contentment, including imagery, muscle,
and meditation exercises. When effective, these techniques create
conditions conducive to experiencing contentment, inner calmness
and feelings of "oneness" or connection. These positive
emotional states are in turn accompanied by reductions in physiological
arousal. By inducing contentment and its associated broadening,
relaxation therapies undo negative emotional arousal.
Relaxation therapies as prevention techniques. Perhaps
most significantly, the benefits of relaxation therapies do
not end once presenting symptoms are treated. Relaxation therapies
also double as prevention techniques. Relaxation therapy offers
repeated practice at self-initiated contentment. This practice
changes people—it builds their personal resources. For instance,
a recent experiment with medical students demonstrated that
mindfulness meditation not only decreases anxiety and depression,
but also increases empathy and spirituality (Shapiro et al.,
1998). So, people who practice relaxation techniques not only
gain practical skills for managing subsequent stressors, but
also develop more complex and resilient views of self (Benson
et al., 1994; Kabat-Zinn, 1990), improve
their immune functioning (O'Leary, 1990), and even extend their
lives (Alexander et al., 1989). These new sources of resiliency,
according to the broaden–and–build model, function as personal
resources that can be drawn on later to promote coping and enhance
health and well-being.
Finding
Positive Meaning
Contentment,
I have argued, may be particularly useful for counteracting problems
that stem from fear and anxiety. I turn now to depression and
dysphoria, problems that many theorists
have conceptualized as deficits of positive affect (Davidson,
1993; Watson, Clark, & Carey, 1988). In my view, a wide range
of positive emotions should be useful for counteracting depressive
tendencies. I trace the origins of this idea in the sections to
come. I begin with early behavioral therapies centered on increasing
rates of pleasant activities, and follow the movement toward more
cognitive therapies centered on explanatory style. Behavioral
and cognitive therapies have each been tested as methods for treating
and preventing depression. Both types have been shown to be effective.
Even so, I think the pivotal role of positive emotions has been
underdeveloped within each approach. To better deploy positive
emotions, I propose that strategies for counteracting depression
incorporate findings from the emerging literature on coping styles
marked by finding positive meaning.
Early emphasis on pleasant activities. Building on behavioral
theories, Lewinsohn and colleagues
postulated that depression may result in part from a deficit
of response-contingent positive reinforcement (for a review,
see Lewinsohn & Gotlib, 1995). To
treat depression, these researchers developed a set of intervention
strategies (including training in assertiveness, social skills,
relaxation, decision-making and time management) aimed at decreasing
the intensity and frequency of depressed persons' unpleasant
events and at increasing their rates of engagement in pleasant
activities (e.g., Brown & Lewinsohn,
1984; Lewinsohn, Sullivan, & Grosscup,
1980). To monitor changes in rates of pleasant activities, these
researchers used the 320-item Pleasant Events Schedule (PES;
MacPhillamy & Lewinsohn,
1982) that taps a vast array of activities, including various
forms of socializing, being in nature, being creative, being
physically active, and other forms of leisure. Research has
demonstrated that Lewinsohn's and
related behavioral therapies for depression are effective: They
increase engagement in pleasant activities and decrease levels
of depression (see Lewinsohn &
Gotlib, 1995, for a review). A focus
on increasing pleasant activities also appears to prevent the
initial onset of depressive symptoms (Munoz, Ying, Armas,
Chan, & Guzza, 1987). Despite these successes in both treating and
preventing depression, researchers have been unable to locate
the precise mechanisms responsible for documented therapeutic
benefits (Lewinsohn & Gotlib,
1995). Nor have they been able to clearly demonstrate a causal
relationship between rates of pleasant activities and subsequent
levels of depression (see, e.g., Hoevenaars
& van Son, 1990; Lewinsohn &
Hoberman, 1982). This raises the possibility that increased
rates of pleasant activities may be a sign of depression remission
and perhaps not a cause of it.
Again,
as with relaxation therapies, it is useful to recognize that behavioral
therapies centered on increasing pleasant activities originated
within an era of psychology that ignored emotion-related concepts.
Perhaps this explains why pleasant activities are emphasized over
pleasant subjective experiences, like positive emotions. Although
it seems obvious that pleasant activities should produce positive
emotions, it also bears underscoring that individuals vary in
their likelihood of experiencing positive emotions in response
to pleasant activities (Rose & Staats,
1988; Langston, 1994). Correlational
studies have shown, for example, that individuals are more likely
to experience positive emotions following pleasant events if they
perceive control over those events, or mark, celebrate, or otherwise
share those events with others (Bryant, 1989; Langston, 1994).
Perhaps even more critically, individuals also vary in their likelihood
of experiencing positive emotions in response to unpleasant events
(Lyubomirsky & Tucker, 1998; Kuiper
& Martin, 1998; Martin & Lefcourt,
1983 ; Nezu et al., 1988), even those as extreme as caregiving and bereavement (Folkman,
1997; Folkman, Moskowitz,
Ozer, & Park, 1997; Stein et al., 1997). These observations
render an exclusive focus on the valence of events and activities
misleading.
Contemporary emphasis on explanatory style. More recently,
reflecting the influence of cognitive approaches to emotion and
therapy, theoretical focus has shifted away from day-to-day events
and activities themselves and toward the meanings individuals
construct from them. For instance, reformulated learned helplessness
theory (Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978) posited that
it is not bad events per se that lead to depression, but rather
the habitual style in which individuals explain their bad events.
Explanations based on internal, stable, and global causes have
been shown to be depressogenic (
Peterson & Seligman, 1984). Cognitive interventions
spotlight these pessimistic explanatory styles, along with negative
beliefs, and seek to change them. Clinical studies have documented
that such cognitive therapies are effective—both for treating
and preventing depression. Moreover, their efficacy has been shown
to be mediated by changes in explanatory style ( DeRubeis et al., 1990; Gillham, Reivich, Jaycox, & Seligman, 1995; Seligman et al., 1988). That
is, learning to explain bad events in circumscribed ways—with
reference to external, unstable, and specific causes (also called
learned optimism) —alleviates and prevents depression.
So,
the track record of cognitive therapies centered on explanatory
style surpasses that of behavioral therapies centered on pleasant
activities. This alone underscores that the emphasis switch
from the valence of activities to the meanings people construct
from them is crucial. Even so, at present most cognitive therapies
strive to cultivate "non-negative thinking" (Seligman,
1990). In other words, even though this type of learned optimism
can counteract depression, it may do little to increase experiences
of positive emotion or optimize well-being. Again, these positive
states are not merely the absence of negative states. To take
this recognition seriously, psychologists must push the analysis
of meaning-making beyond the traditional boundaries of explanatory
style. (For a compatible critique of learned
optimism, see Peterson, 1998.)
Emerging emphasis on finding positive meaning. Of particular
interest in this effort is the emerging literature on the ways
individuals seek and find positive meaning (e.g., Affleck
& Tennen, 1996; Folkman,
1997; Folkman et al., 1997; Stein et
al., 1997). Holding spiritual or religious beliefs or otherwise
appreciating the "meaning of life" on philosophical
levels can increase people's likelihood of finding positive meaning
(Folkman, 1997; Frankl,
1959; Thompson & Janigian, 1988). Yet with or without the infusion of religion,
people find positive meaning in daily life through multiple pathways.
These include: (a) Reframing adverse events in a positive light
(also called positive reappraisal); (b) infusing ordinary events
with positive value; and (c) pursing and attaining realistic goals.
Daily experiences of positive meaning also come in several forms.
The most frequently reported forms include: feeling connected
to others and cared about (22%), having an opportunity to be distracted
from everyday cares (21%), feeling a sense of achievement, pride,
or self-esteem (17%), feeling hope or optimism (13%), and receiving
affirmation or validation from others (11%). (Percentages indicate
the proportion of each form of positive meaning across 215 positive
meaningful events described by a sample of 36 caregivers; see
Folkman et al., 1997, Table 2.)
Finding
positive meaning in daily life appears to have important psychological
repercussions. For instance, Folkman
and colleagues have found that daily sources of positive meaning
predict recovery from depressed mood and long-term psychological
well-being (Folkman, Chesney, Collette, Boccellari, &
Cooke, 1996). Similar to finding positive meaning in ordinary
daily events, finding positive meaning in major life events (e.g.,
serious medical problems or the death of a loved one) has also
been shown to predict long- term psychological well-being and
health (Affleck & Tennen, 1996;
Davis, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Larson,
1998 ). Thus, finding positive meaning outperforms engaging
in pleasant activities as a predictor of depression remission
and future psychological well-being. How does positive meaning
work?
Positive
meaning elicits positive emotions. To answer this question,
it is critical to recognize that positive meaning and positive
emotions go hand in hand. Bereaved caregivers' daily success at
finding positive meaning, Folkman and colleagues have shown, predicts their experiences
of positive emotions (Folkman, 1997;
Moskowitz, Folkman, Collette, &
Vittinghoff, 1996; for related findings
on daily benefit-reminding, see Affleck & Tennen,
1996). Moreover, different forms of positive meaning are likely
to yield different types of positive emotions. For instance, feeling
connected to others and cared for may coincide with love or contentment,
whereas being distracted from everyday cares may coincide with
interest or joy. Importantly, it appears that positive emotions
"may not need to be either intense or prolonged to produce
a beneficial effect" (Folkman,
1997, p. 1218). This observation reminds us that even the most
subtle positive emotions merit empirical attention.
I
propose that the broadening effects associated with experiences
of positive emotions explain the therapeutic benefit attributed
to finding positive meaning. Again, the broaden– and–build model
suggests that the momentarily broadened thought–action repertoire
characteristic of positive emotions is psychologically incompatible
with the narrowed thought–action repertoire characteristic of
negative emotions. Positive emotions, the broaden–and–build
model holds, open people's mindsets, enabling creative and flexible
thinking. Consistent with this view, Stein et al.(1997) found that people who experienced positive emotion
during bereavement were more likely to have developed abstract,
long-term plans and goals. Together with positive emotions,
plans and goals predicted greater psychological well-being 12
months postbereavement (Stein et al.,
1997). To be sure, these findings are correlational,
and additional studies are needed to test whether positive emotions
produced planning and goal-setting or vice versa. Even so, the
broaden–and–build model predicts that the broadening effects
of positive emotions produced the flexible mental state that
enabled individuals to break out of their negative mindsets
to create and pursue abstract and long-term goals. Finding positive
meaning in adverse circumstances may thus be another case of
positive emotions undoing negative emotions.
Positive
meaning: Treatment and prevention possibilities. Certainly,
momentary relief from sad or depressed mood is not sufficient
to prevent or treat depression. Even so, the broaden– and–build
model predicts that experiences of positive emotions can accumulate
and compound: The psychological broadening sparked by one positive
emotion can increase an individual's receptiveness to subsequent
pleasant or meaningful events, increasing the odds that the individual
will find positive meaning in these subsequent events and experience
additional positive emotions. This can in turn trigger an "upward
spiral" that might, over time, lessen depressive symptoms.
(For a compatible discussion of an upward spiral,
see Aspinwall, 1998.) Thus, through
incremental processes attributable to psychological broadening,
experiences of positive emotions might, over time, facilitate
coping and alleviate depressed mood. Also with time and repeated
experience, the broaden–and–build model predicts that positive
emotions increment people's enduring personal resources. These
may include both intraindividual resources,
like increased psychological and physical resilience, and interpersonal
resources, like enhanced social relationships, which can be the
locus of both pleasant activities and positive meaning. Taken
together, these resources—gained through positive emotion experiences—can
enhance health and well-being.
Exploring
the Range of Benefits
I
have focused thus far on the ways positive emotions might be
tapped to prevent and treat anxiety and depression and thereby
optimize health and well-being. This choice does not mark the
boundary conditions of the benefits of positive emotions. Indeed,
I make the more general claim that the broadening effects of
positive emotions can counteract the narrowing effects of negative
emotions. This dynamic should hold regardless of whether the
problematic negative emotion is fear, anxiety, sadness, anger,
disgust, or some related negative state. It should also hold
regardless of whether the intervening positive emotion is contentment,
joy, interest, love, or some related positive state. To illustrate
this range, I will describe an early social psychology experiment
that tested ways to reduce aggression by inducing "incompatible"
responses.
On
hot summer afternoons, Baron (1976) had a confederate motorist
delay male motorists for 15 seconds after a traffic light turned
green. He did this under one of five conditions. In a distraction
condition, a female confederate pedestrian crossed the street
between the confederate's and subject's cars. In an empathy condition,
the same confederate hobbled along the same route on crutches.
In a humor condition, she crossed wearing an outlandish, humorous
clown mask. In a sexual interest condition, she crossed wearing
"an extremely brief and revealing outfit of a very unusual
type" (Baron, 1976, p. 266). Finally, in a control condition,
no pedestrian crossed the street. The dependent measure of aggression
was horn-honking, measured both by latency to honk and proportion
of motorists honking. I focus here on the behaviors of drivers
exposed to the hot weather (i.e., those who drove vehicles without
air conditioning) because research has shown that uncomfortable
heat increases aggressive tendencies (e.g., Kenrick
& MacFarlane, 1986). These data revealed that those in the
empathy, humor, and sexual interest conditions were significantly
slower and less likely to honk than those who were in the control
and distraction conditions.
Interestingly,
Baron (1976) neither discussed the commonalties across the experimental
conditions nor speculated about possible mechanisms responsible
for this reduced aggression. He simply described the experimentally
induced states as "incompatible" with aggression, a
sidestep that patterns earlier accounts of why relaxation successfully
treats anxiety (Benson, 1975 ; Wolpe, 1958). Examining this
experiment from the vantage point of the broaden–and–build model,
it seems clear that the commonality across experimental conditions
was some variant of positive emotion, and the mechanism underlying
the noted incompatibility was broadening. The momentarily broadened
thought–action repertoire associated with empathy, amusement,
or interpersonal interest could explain the participants' reduced
likelihood of acting on the narrow impulse to honk angrily.
Stepping
off from these findings, it may be that building empathy between
people and groups works to reduce prejudice, aggression, and violence
(Bridgeman, 1981; Feshbach
& Feshbach, 1982) because it taps
into the broadening effects of love and builds social alliances
and bonds. Likewise, invoking amusement and laughter may work
to de-escalate anger and interpersonal conflict (Gottman,
1998; R. E. Smith, 1973) as well as to combat stress and illness
(Cousins, 1985; Fry, 1994; Kuiper &
Martin, 1998; Stone, Neale, Cox, & Napoli, 1994)
because it taps into the broadening effects joy and builds social
bonds and coping resources. Experiences of flow and intrinsic
motivation may similarly work to improve the quality of life (Csikszentmihalyi,
1990; Fredrickson, in press) and foster psychological development
(Csikszentmihalyi & Rathunde,
1998; Deci et al., 1991) because they
tap into the broadening effects of interest, and build intellectual
resources. These possibilities deserve close empirical scrutiny.
The
disparate intervention techniques discussed here— relaxation training,
finding positive meaning, invoking empathy, amusement, or interest—share
the ability to evoke positive emotions while alleviating negative
emotions. This commonality begins to unearth the elusive mechanism
that may be responsible for each technique's demonstrated effectiveness.
People have long held the intuition that positive emotions are
somehow incompatible with negative emotions. Therapists and researchers
and have long operated on this intuition, either explicitly or
implicitly. But exactly how are they incompatible? At what level?
I use the broaden–and–build model to suggest that the fundamental
incompatibility resides at the level of the breadth of the momentary
thought–action repertoire. During most positive emotions that
repertoire is broad, yielding flexible, receptive, and to some
degree unpredictable thinking and action. During most negative
emotions that repertoire is narrow, yielding fixated, less receptive,
and more predictable thinking and action. Moreover, the
broaden–and– build model holds that an emotion's thought–action
repertoire directs changes in physiological activity. If a specific
action tendency is called forth, then physiological changes are
mobilized to support that action tendency (Levenson, 1994). If, by contrast, a broadened
thought–action repertoire is called forth, then physiological
activity moves toward midrange or relaxed levels to support a
wide range of thoughts and actions. Positive emotions and
negative emotions are fundamentally incompatible because a person's
thought– action repertoire cannot be simultaneously broad and
narrow. This incompatibility, I suggest, accounts for the undoing
effect of positive emotions.
A
mixed set of intervention strategies, then, can be woven together
by the idea that each is effective because it capitalizes on positive
emotions. Put differently, the broaden–and–build model provides
a parsimonious explanation for how these distinct intervention
strategies work to alleviate negative emotions. At present, this
proposal remains a set of theory-based hypotheses that need to
be put to empirical test. Rigorous tests will include measures
sensitive to subtle changes in emotions and the breadth of momentary
thought– action repertoires as well as health and well-being.
Such future studies will be necessary to confirm, modify, or discard
my proposals regarding common underlying mechanisms.
Future Directions
If
future tests uphold the predictions advanced here, will existing
intervention strategies be discarded in favor of techniques
that invoke positive emotions directly? Not a chance. For the
simple reason alluded to earlier: Emotions cannot be instilled
directly. Typically, emotions follow from appraisals of the
personal meaning of daily events. Atypically, emotions can be
"jump-started" by introducing an isolated component
of the more complex emotion system. In any case, when people
wish to willfully alter their emotional states, they must necessarily
do so indirectly. (For a review of emotion
regulation techniques, see Gross, 1998.) The intervention
strategies I have discussed here represent effective methods
for inducing positive emotions. What the present analysis does
suggest is that fruitful avenues for discovering additional
intervention strategies—or fine-tuning existing ones–will follow
positive emotions. Finding ways to cultivate positive emotions
will forge paths towards health and well-being.
Given
that emotions typically follow from assessments of personal meaning,
a look to factors that predispose individuals to find positive
meaning is warranted. The personality traits of optimism, hopefulness,
and happiness appear relevant. Individuals who possess these traits,
studies have shown, tend to be the ones most likely to find positive
meaning (Davis et al., 1998; Lyubomirsky
& Tucker, 1998), experience positive emotions (Affleck &
Tennen, 1996; Scheier & Carver,
1992; Seidlitz, Wyer,
& Diener, 1997), evaluate self-relevant
information carefully and with less defensiveness ( Aspinwall,
1998; Aspinwall & Brunhart,
1996; Trope & Pomerantz, 1998),
employ effective coping strategies (Aspinwall
& Taylor, 1997; Fontaine, Manstead,
& Wagner, 1993; Irving, Snyder, & Crowson,
1998; Scheier, Weintraub,
& Carver, 1986; Snyder et al., 1991), and experience fastest
relief from distress (Davis et al., 1998; Folkman, 1997; Scheier et al., 1989).
Importantly,
the extent to which people hold positive outlooks, although typically
stable, can be enhanced. Psychoeducational
programs, based on cognitive therapies for depression, have
been demonstrated to produce "learned optimism" (Gillham
et al., 1995; Seligman, 1990). Finding positive meaning in dire
circumstances may be another route to increasing levels of optimism:
Studies have documented that finding benefit in adversity is not
only predicted by preexisting levels of optimism but is also predictive
of future increases in optimism (Davis et al. 1998; Park, Cohen,
& Murch, 1996). Increased optimism,
however attained, should translate into an increased ability to
find positive meaning and experience positive emotions in daily
life. Experiences of positive emotions, in turn, should broaden
habitual modes of thinking and build personal resources for coping
with life's adversity. Positive emotions are likely to be the
active ingredient that energizes this upward spiral that optimizes
health and well-being.
References
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman,
M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978).
Learned helplessness in people: Critique and
reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87,
49–7 Affleck,
G., & Tennen, H. (1996). Construing
benefits from adversity: Adaptational
significance and dispositional underpinnings. Journal of
Personality, 64, 899–922Alexander, C. N., Langer, E. J.,
Newman, R. I., Chandler, H. M., & Davies, J. L. (1989).
Transcendental meditation, mindfulness, and longevity: An experimental
study with the elderly. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 57, 950–964. Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U.
(in press). A neuropsychological
theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition.
Psychological Review.Aspinwall, L. G. (1998).
Rethinking the role of positive affect in
self-regulation. Motivation and
Emotion, 22, 1–32 Aspinwall, L. G., & Brunhart, S. M. (1996).
Distinguishing optimism from denial: Optimistic beliefs predict
attention to health threats. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 993–1003.Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1997).
A stitch in time: Self-regulation and proactive coping. Psychological
Bulletin, 121 ,
417–436.Barefoot, J. C., Dahlstrom, W. G., & Williams, R. B., Jr. (1983). Hostility,
CHD incidence, and total mortality: A 25-year follow-up study
of 255 physicians. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45, 59–63.
Baron, R. A. (1976).
The reduction of human aggression: A field study of the influence
of incompatible reactions. Journal of Applied
Social Psychology, 6, 260–274. Benson, H. (1975). The relaxation response. New York: Morrow.Benson, H., Kornhaber, A., Kornhaber,
C., LeChanu, M. N., Zuttermeister,
P. C., Myers, P., & Friedman, R. (1994). Increases
in positive psychological characteristics with a new relaxation-response
curriculum in high school students. Journal of Research
and Development in Education, 27, 226–231. Bernstein, D.
A., & Borkovec, T. D. (1973).
Progressive relaxation training: A manual for the helping
professions. Champaign, Illinois: Research Press.Blumenthal, J. A. (1985). Relaxation therapy, biofeedback, and
behavioral medicine. Psychotherapy, 22, 516–530.
Boulton, M. J., & Smith, P. K. (1992). The social nature of play fighting and play chasing: Mechanisms and strategies
underlying cooperation and compromise. In J. H. Barkow,
L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby
(Eds.), The adapted mind:
Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp.
429–444). New York: Oxford University Press.Bridgeman, D. (1981). Enhanced role-taking through cooperative
interdependence: A field study. Child Development,
52, 1231–1238.Brown, . A.,
& Lewinsohn, P. M. (1984).
A psychoeducational approach to the treatment of depression:
Comparison of group, individual, and
minimal contact procedures. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52, 774–783.
Bryant, F. B. (1989). A four-factor model of perceived control: Avoiding, coping, obtaining,
and savoring. Journal of Personality, 57,
773–797. Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of human mating. New York:
Basic Books. Cabanac, M. (1971,September 17).Physiological role of
pleasure. Science, 173, 1103–1107. Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R.,
& Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes: II. Arm flexion and extension have
differential effects on attitudes. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17.
Chen, Y., & Dilsaver, C. C. (1996). Lifetime rates of suicide attempts among subjects with bipolar and unipolar disorders relative to subjects without Axis 1 disorders.
Biological Psychiatry, 39, 896–899. Coryell, W., Scheftner, W., Keller, M., & Endicott, J. (1993). The enduring psychosocial consequences of mania and
depression. American Journal of
Psychiatry, 150, 720–727. Cousins,
N. (1985). Therapeutic
value of laughter. Integrative Psychiatry,
3, 112. Csikszentmihalyi,
M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of
optimal experience. New York: Harper Perennial.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. & Rathunde, K. (1998). The development
of the person: An experiential perspective on the ontogenesis
of psychological complexity. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) &
R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol.
1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp.
635–684). New York: Wiley.Davidson, R. J. (1993). The neuropsychology
of emotion and affective style. In
M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.),
Handbook of emotion (pp. 143–154). New York: Guilford
Press.Davis, C. G., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Larson, J.
(1998). Making sense of loss and benefiting from experience:
Two construals of meaning. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75 , 561–574.Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L.
G., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). Motivation and education: Theself-determination
perspective. Educational Psychologist, 26, 325–346.de Rivera, J., Possel, L.,
Verette, J. A., & Weiner, B. (1989). Distinguishing
elation, gladness, and joy. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 57, 1015–1023. DeRubeis, R. J., Evans, M. D., Hollon, S. D., Garvey,
M. J., Grove, W. M., & Tuason,
V. B. (1990). How does cognitive therapy work? Cognitive change
and symptom change in cognitive therapy and pharmacotherapy
for depression. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 58, 862–869.Dolhinow, P. J., & Bishop,
N. (1970). The development
of motor skills and social relationships among primates through
play. In J. P. Hill (Ed.), Minnesota
Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 141–198).
Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition
and Emotion, 6, 169–200. Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983,
September 16). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes
among emotions. Science, 221, 1208–1210.Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith,
C. A. (1988). Shades of joy: Patterns of
appraisal differentiating pleasant emotions. Cognition and
Emotion, 2, 301–331. Estrada,
C. A., Isen, A. M., & Young, M.
J. (1997). Positive affect facilitates integration of information and decreases anchoring
in reasoning among physicians. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72, 117–135.Eysenck, H. J. (1994). Cancer, personality and stress: Predictions and prevention. Advances
in Behavioral Research and Therapy, 16, 167–215.Feshbach, N. D., & Feshbach,
S. (1982). Empathy training and the regulation of aggression:
Potentialities and limitations. Academic
Psychological Bulletin, 4, 399–413.Folkman,
S. (1997). Positive psychological states
and coping with severe stress. Social Science Medicine, 45,
1207–1221. Folkman,
S., Chesney, M. A., Collette, L.,
Boccellari, A., & Cooke, M. (1996).
Post-bereavement depressive mood and its pre- bereavement predictors
in HIV+ and HIV- gay men. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 70, 336–348.Folkman S., Moskowitz, J. T., Ozer, E. M., & Park, C. L. (1997). Positive meaningful
events and coping in the context of HIV/AIDS. In B. H. Gottlieb
(Ed.) Coping with chronic stress (pp. 293–314). New York:
Plenum.Fontaine, K. R., Manstead, A. S., & Wagner, H. (1993).
Optimism, perceived control over stress, and
coping. European Journal of Personality, 7, 267–281.Frankl, V. E. (1959). Man's search for meaning: An introduction to logotherapy.
New York: Simon & Schuster.Fredrickson, B. L. (in press). Extracting meaning from past affective experiences:
The importance of peaks, ends, and specific emotions. Cognition
and Emotion.Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General
Psychology, 2, 300–319.Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson,
R. W. (1998). Positive emotions speed recovery
from the cardiovascular sequelae of
negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion,
12, 191–220.Fredrickson, B. L., Mancuso,
R. A., Branigan, C., & Tugade,
M. (1999). The undoing
effect of positive emotions. Manuscript submitted
for publication. Fredrickson,
B. L., Maynard, K. E., Helms, M. J., Haney, T. L., Siegler,
I. C., & Barefoot, J. C. (1999). Hostility predicts magnitude
and duration of blood pressure response to anger. Manuscript
submitted for publication. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge,
England: Cambridge University Press. Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., &
Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action
readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
57, 212–228. Fry, W. F. (1994). The biology of humor. Humor: International Journal of Humor
Research, 7, 111–126.Futterman, A. D., Kemeny, M. E., Shapiro, D.,
& Fahey, J. L. (1994). Immunological andphysiological
changes associated with induced positive and negative mood.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 56, 499–511.Gillham, J. E., Reivich, K. J., Jaycox, L. H., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1995). Prevention
of depressive symptoms in schoolchildren: Two-year follow-up.
Psychological Science, 6, 343–351.Gottman, J. M. (1998). Psychology and the study of marital processes.
Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 169–197Greer, S., & Morris, T.
(1975). Psychological attributes of
women who develop breast cancer: A controlled study. Journal
of Psychosomatic Research, 19, 147–153. Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative
review. Review of General Psychology, 2,
271–299.Hazen,
N. L., & Durrett, M. E. (1982). Relationship of security of attachment and cognitive
mapping abilities in 2-year-olds. Developmental
Psychology, 18, 751–759.Hoevenaars, J., & van Son, M. J. M. (1990). New chances for Lewinsohn's
social reinforcement theory of depression. In
H. G. Zapotocsky & T. Wenzel (Eds.) The scientific dialogue:
From basic research to clinical intervention (pp. 65–70).
Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger.Irving, L. M., Snyder, C. R., Crowson, & J. J.
(1998). Hope and caring with cancer by college
women. Journal of Personality, 66, 195–214.Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior.
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 203–253.Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of
affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 47, 1206–1217. Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., &
Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–1131. Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M. S., Mertz, E., & Robinson,
G. F. (1985).
The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1413–1426.
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.Jacobson, E. (1938). Progressive relaxation. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.Janoff-Bulman, R., & Frantz, C. M. (1997). The impact of trauma on meaning: From
meaningless world to meaningful life. In M. Power & C. R.
Brewin (Eds.) The transformation
of meaning in psychological therapies (pp. 91–106). New
York: Wiley.Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body
and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Delta.Kabat-Zinn, J., Massion, A. O., Kristeller,
J., Peterson, L. G., Fletcher, K. E., Pbert,
L., Lenderking, W. R., & Santorelli,
S. F. (1992). Effectiveness of a meditation-based
stress reduction program in the treatment of anxiety disorders.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 149 ,
936–943.Kaplan, S. (1992). Environmental preference in a knowledge-seeking, knowledge-using organism.
In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.)
The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the
generation of culture (pp. 581–598). New York: Oxford University
Press.Kaplan, S. (1995). The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative
framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology,
15, 169–182.Keltner,
D., & Bonanno, G. A. (1997). A study of laughter and dissociation: Distinct correlates of laughter
and smiling during bereavement. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 73, 687–702. Kenrick, D. T., & MacFarlane, S. W. (1986). Ambient temperature and horn honking: A field study of the heat/aggression
relationship. Environment and Behavior,
18, 179–191.Kuiper, N. A., & Martin,
R. A. (1998). Laughter and stress in daily
life: Relation to positive and negative affect. Motivation
and Emotion, 22, 133–153. Langer, E. J. (1989). Mindfulness. Reading,
MA: Addison-Wesley.
Langer,
E. J. (1992). Matters of mind: Mindfulness/mindlessness in perspective.
Consciousness and Cognition, 1, 289–305.
Langston,
C. A. (1994). Capitalizing on and coping with daily-life events:
Expressive responses to positive events. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1112–1125.Larsen, R. J., Kasimatis,
M., & Frey, K. (1992).
Facilitating the furrowed brow: An unobtrusive test of the facial
feedback hypothesis applied to unpleasant affect. Cognition
and Emotion, 6, 321–338.Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford
University Press.Lemerise, E. A., & Dodge, K. A. (1993). The development of
anger and hostile interactions. In
M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.),
Handbook of emotions (pp. 537–546). New York:
Guilford Press.Levenson, R. W. (1992). Autonomic nervous system differences
among emotions. Psychological Science, 3, 23–27.
Levenson, R. W. (1994). Human emotions: A functional view. In P. Ekman & R. Davidson, (Eds.), The
nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (pp. 123–126).
New York: Oxford University Press. Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L.,
Friesen, W. V., & Ekman, P. (1991). Emotion, physiology, and expression in old age.
Psychology and Aging, 6, 28–35.Levenson, R. W., Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Voluntary facial action generates emotion-specific autonomic nervous system
activity. Psychophysiology, 27, 363–384. Lewinsohn, P. M., & Gotlib, I.
H. (1995). Behavioral
theory and treatment of depression. In
E. E. Beckham & W. R. Leber (Eds.),
Handbook of depression (2nd ed., pp. 352–375).
New York: Guilford.Lewinsohn, P. M., & Hoberman, H. (1982). Depression. In A. Bellack, M. Hersen, & A. Kazdin (Eds.),
International Handbook of Behavioral Modification and Therapy.
New York: Plenum.Lewinsohn, P. M., Sullivan, J. M., & Grosscup, S.
J. (1980). Changing reinforcing events: An
approach to the treatment of depression. Psychotherapy:
Theory, Research, and Practice, 47, 322–334.Lyubomirsky, S., & Tucker, K. L. (1998). Implications
of individual differences in subjective happiness for perceiving,
interpreting, and thinking about life events. Motivation
and Emotion, 22, 155–186.MacPhillamy, D. J., & Lewinsohn,
P. M. (1982). The Pleasant Events Schedule:
Studies on reliability, validity, and scale intercorrelation.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 50, 363–380.Markus, H. R., & Kitayama,
S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition,
emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.Martin, R. A., Kuiper, N.
A., Olinger, J., & Dance, K. A.
(1993). Humor, coping with stress, self-concept, and psychological well-being.
Humor, 6, 89–104.Martin, R. A., & Lefcourt, H. M. (1983). Sense of humor as a moderator of the relation between
stressors and moods. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1313–1324.Miller, J. J., Fletcher, K., & Kabat-Zinn, J. (1995). Three-year follow-up and clinical implications of a mindfulness meditation-based
stress reduction intervention in the treatment of anxiety disorders.
General Hospital Psychiatry, 17, 192–200.Moskowitz, J. T., Folkman, S., Collette, L., & Vittinghoff,
E. (1996). Coping and
mood during AIDS-related caregiving
and bereavement. Annals of Behavioral
Medicine, 18, 49–57.Munoz,
R. F., Ying, Y. W., Armas, R., Chan,
F., & Guzza, R. (1987). The San Francisco Depression Prevention Project: A randomized trial with
medical outpatients. In R. F. Munoz (Ed.), Depression prevention:
Research directions (pp. 199–215). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.Nezu, A. M., Nezu, C. M., & Blissett, S. E. (1988). Sense of humor as a moderator of the
relation between stressful events and psychological distress:
A prospective analysis. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 54, 520–525.Nolen-Hoeksema,
S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and the duration of episodes of depressed
mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 20–28.
Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotions. Cambridge, MA:
Blackwell. Ohman, A. (1993). Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: Clinical phenomenology,
evolutionary perspectives, and information-processing mechanisms.
In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.),
Handbook of emotions (pp. 511–536). New York:
Guilford Press.O'Leary, A. (1990). Stress, emotion, and human immune function.
Psychological Bulletin, 108, 363–382.Orians, G. H., & Heerwagen, J. H. (1992). Evolved responses to landscapes. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted
mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture
(pp. 555–579). New York: Oxford University Press.Panksepp, J. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorders, psychostimulants, and intolerance of childhood playfulness:
A tragedy in the making? Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 7 , 91–98. Park, C., Cohen, L., &
Murch, R. (1996). Assessment and prediction of stress-related growth.
Journal of Personality, 64, 71–105.Peterson, C. (1998). The future of optimism. Unpublished manuscript, University of Michigan.Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence.
Psychological Review, 91, 347–374.Renninger, K. A., Hidi, S., & Krapp, A. (Eds.). (1992). The role of interest in learning
and development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Rose, G. D., & Staats, A. W. (1988). Depression
and the frequency and strength of pleasant events: Exploration
of the Staats- Heiby
theory. Behavioural Research and Theory, 26, 489–494.Rosenberg, E. L. (1998). Levels of analysis and the organization of affect.
Review of General Psychology, 2, 247–270. Ruch, W. (1993). Exhilaration and humor. In
M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.),
Handbook of emotions (pp. 605–616). New York:
Guilford Press.Ryff, C. D., & Singer, B. (1998). Contours of positive
human health. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 1–28.
Scheier, M. F., & Bridges, M. W. (1995). Person variables and health: Personality predispositions and acute psychological
states as shared determinants of disease. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 57, 255–268.Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1992). Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-being: Theoretical
overview and empirical update. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
16, 201–228. Scheier, M. F., Matthews, K. A., Owens,
J. F., Magovern, G. J., Lefebvre,
C., Abbott, R. A., & Carver, C. S. (1989). Dispositional
optimism and recovery from coronary artery bypass surgery: The
beneficial effects on physical and psychological well-being.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
57, 1024–1040.Scheier, M. F., Weintraub,
J. K., & Carver, C. S. (1986). Coping with stress: Divergent strategies of optimists
and pessimists. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 51, 1257–1264.Seidlitz, L., Wyer, R. S.,
& Diener, E. (1997). Cognitive correlates of subjective well-being: The processing of valenced life events by happy and unhappy persons. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 240–256.Seligman, M. E. P. (1990). Learned optimism: How to change your mind and your
life. New York: Knopf.Seligman, M. E. P. (1998, October). What is the 'good life'? APA Monitor,
p. 2. Seligman,
M. E. P., Castellon, C., Cacciola,
J., Schulman, P., Luborsky, L., Ollove,
M., & Downing, R. (1988). Explanatory style change during cognitive therapy for
unipolar depression. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 1–6.Selye, H. (1956). The stress of life. New York: McGraw-Hill.Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G. E., & Bonner, G. (1998). Effects of mindfulness-based
stress reduction on medical and premedical students. Journal
of Behavioral Medicine, 21, 581–599. Smith, J. C. (1990). Cognitive-behavioral relaxation training: A new
system of strategies for treatment and assessment. New York:
Springer.Smith, R. E. (1973). The use of humor in the counterconditioning
of anger responses: A case study. Behavior Therapy, 4,
576–580.Snyder, C. R., Harris, C., Anderson, J. R., Holleran, S. A., Irving, L. M., Sigmon,
S. T., Yoshinobu, L., Gibb, J., Langelle,
C., & Harney, P. (1991). The will and the ways: Development
and validation of an individual-differences measure of hope.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 570–585.
Solomon, R. L. (1980). The opponent-process theory of acquired motivation:
The costs of pleasure and benefits of pain. American Psychologist,
35, 691–712. Stanislavski, C. (1965). An actor prepares. New York: Theater Arts
Books. (Original work published 1948)Stein, N. L., Folkman, S.,
Trabasso, T., & Richards, T. A. (1997). Appraisal and goal processes as predictors of psychological well-being
in bereaved caregivers. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 72, 872–884.Stepper, S., & Strack,
F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional
and nonemotional feelings.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 211–220.Stone, A. A., Neale, J. M., Cox, D. S., & Napoli,
A. (1994). Daily events are associated
with a secretory immune response to
an oral antigen in men. Health Psychology,
13, 440–418. Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating
conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive
test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.Thompson, S. C., & Janigian, A. S. (1988). Life schemes: A framework for understanding the search for meaning. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7, 260–280.Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness: Vol. I. The positive affects. New
York: Springer. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). The past explains the present:
Emotional adaptations and the structure of ancestral environments.
Ethology and Sociobiology, 11, 375–424. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological
foundations of culture. In J. H. Barkow,
L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby
(Eds.) The adapted mind:
Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp.
19–136). New York: Oxford University Press.Trope, Y., & Pomerantz, E. M. (1998). Resolving conflicts among self-evaluative motives: Positive experiences
as a resource for overcoming defensiveness. Motivation
and Emotion, 22, 53–72.Ulrich, R. S., Simons, R. E., Losito, B. D.,
Fiorito, E., Miles, M. A., & Zelson,
M. (1991). Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban
environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 11, 201–230.Watson, D., Clark, L. A., &
Carey, G. (1988).
Positive and negative affectivity and their relations to anxiety and
depressive disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
97, 346–353. Williams,
R. B., Jr., Haney, L. T., Lee, K. L., Kong, Y., Blumenthal,
J., & Whalen, R. (1980). Type A
behavior, hostility, and coronary atherosclerosis. Psychosomatic
Medicine, 42, 539–549. Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Stanford, CA:
Stanford University Press.
Footnotes
1 In an earlier article (Fredrickson, 1998), I discussed
how the positive emotion of love can be conceptualized as joy,
interest, and contentment—experienced singly or in combination
or succession—within the context of a specific, and often enduring
interpersonal relationship.
2 The term mindfulness
has been used both within the meditation literature (e.g., Kabat-
Zinn, 1990) and within the social psychological
literature (e.g., Langer, 1989, 1992). An attribute common to
both usages of the term is heightened awareness of both the content
and the context of thinking. A key distinction, however, is that
the mindfulness cultivated by meditation is more tranquil and
less active than the mindfulness described by Langer. For a discussion
of the similarities and differences between meditation and a form
of mindfulness training based on Langer's work, see work by Alexander
et al. (1989).
3 Finding meaning in loss
can be construed both as making sense of the event and as finding
benefit in the experience. Finding positive meaning is construed
as the latter. For discussions on the distinction between these
two construals of meaning, see Davis
et al. (1998), Janoff-Bulman and Frantz
(1997), and Thompson and Janigian (1988).
Barbara L. Fredrickson, Department
of Psychology, Research Center for Group Dynamics, and Women's
Studies Program, University of Michigan.
I thank Jeff Chappell, Susan Nolen-Hoeksema, Christopher Peterson, and Erika Rosenberg for comments
offered on earlier versions of this article.
Correspondence concerning this article
should be addressed to Barbara L. Fredrickson, Department of Psychology,
University of Michigan, 525 East University
Avenue, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48109-1109. E-mail: blf@umich.edu
|
|